|
|
Volume
6: No. 3, July 2009
SPECIAL TOPIC
Developmental Perspectives on Nutrition and Obesity
From Gestation to Adolescence
Layla Esposito, PhD; Jennifer O. Fisher, PhD; Julie A. Mennella, PhD; Deanna M. Hoelscher, PhD, RD, LD, CNS; Terry T. Huang, PhD, MPH
Suggested citation for this article: Esposito L, Fisher JO, Mennella JA, Hoelscher DM, Huang TT. Developmental perspectives on nutrition and obesity from gestation to adolescence. Prev Chronic Dis 2009;6(3):A94.
http://www.cdc.gov/pcd/issues/2009/ jul/09_0014.htm. Accessed [date].
PEER REVIEWED
Abstract
Obesity results from a complex combination of factors that act at many stages throughout a person’s life. Therefore, examining childhood nutrition and obesity from a developmental perspective is warranted. A developmental perspective recognizes the cumulative effects of factors that contribute to eating behavior and obesity, including biological and socioenvironmental factors that are relevant at different stages of development. A developmental perspective considers family, school, and
community context. During gestation, risk factors for obesity include maternal diet, overweight, and smoking. In early childhood, feeding practices, taste acquisition, and eating in the absence of hunger must be considered. As children become more independent during middle childhood and adolescence, school nutrition, food marketing, and social networks become focal points for obesity prevention or intervention. Combining a multilevel approach with a developmental perspective can inform more
effective and sustainable strategies for obesity prevention.
Back to top
Introduction
Obesity results from a combination of factors that occur at different stages during a person’s lifetime. Therefore, childhood nutrition and obesity should be examined from a developmental perspective. First, prenatal and early life experiences influence the trajectory of weight into adulthood (1). Second, during certain critical periods, vulnerabilities are intensified to specific maternal and environmental exposures that can lead to obesity (2). Finally, the cumulative effects of
multiple factors contribute to eating behavior and obesity (3).
Growing evidence suggests that prenatal and maternal interactions and influences must be considered along with biological and environmental variables throughout infancy, childhood, and adolescence that may lead to — or prevent — obesity. Examining nutrition and obesity from a developmental perspective combines social context and biological influences with individual behavior (4,5). Social context can range from family, to school, to the broader community.
We
describe where these contexts interact with biological processes to affect food behavior and obesity. Although a person is at risk for obesity throughout his life, we focus on specific developmental susceptibilities for obesity from gestation through adolescence (Table).
Back to top
Gestational Period
Risk for obesity and metabolic disorders begins during gestation (1). Obesity is linked to in utero exposure to glucocorticoids, protein restrictions, and maternal diet and obesity. Exposing fetal rats to high levels of glucocorticoids reduces birth weight and results in adults with high blood levels of insulin and glucose. Male offspring of female rats with a history of fetal exposure to glucocorticoids also exhibit low birth weight and glucose intolerance — a multigenerational effect
(6).
Feeding low-protein diets to pregnant rats produces a broad spectrum of disorders in their offspring (7): hypertension and vascular defects (8,9), altered fetal pancreatic development and structure (10), altered glucose tolerance (11), altered liver structure and function (12), altered gene expression (13), and possibly type 2 diabetes mellitus (10). In humans, low protein intake by women in late pregnancy has been associated with low birth weight, a marker of risk for obesity and other
metabolic disorders later in life (14).
A maternal diet high in fat also causes long-term harm to the offspring. Female rat pups born to and suckled by fat-fed mothers have high blood pressure, even after being placed on a balanced diet after weaning. The offspring are hypertensive, show vascular changes, and have high blood insulin levels. Such changes in early life are likely to lead to metabolic syndrome in adult animals (15).
Many studies have indicated a link between smoking during pregnancy and the offspring’s subsequent obesity, but the underlying mechanism has not been established. Children born to women who smoke during pregnancy typically weigh less at birth, and they often have a catch-up period during their first year, although studies have not consistently found a link between catch-up growth and greater childhood body mass index (BMI). Other hypotheses postulate mechanisms such as poor placental
blood supply because of nicotine-induced vasoconstriction, poor maternal nutrition, and fetal exposure to carbon monoxide. Whatever the mechanism, the relationship between smoking during pregnancy and children’s overweight is well documented (16,17)
In 1 study, for example, babies born to mothers who smoked during pregnancy weighed less than did babies born to nonsmokers (18). However, as they reached adolescence (age 11 years for girls, 16 for boys), children exposed to tobacco in utero had a significantly greater risk of being in the highest 10% of BMI for their age group. This tendency continued to strengthen with age (participants were followed through age 33) and could not be explained by other factors in their childhood,
adolescence, or adulthood (18).
A recent 27-year study of children whose mothers smoked during pregnancy found larger annual changes in cholesterol levels; high-density lipoprotein cholesterol levels decreased and low-density lipoprotein cholesterol levels increased more than in children not exposed to tobacco in utero. This was the first study to suggest that smoking during pregnancy is linked to adverse changes in the lipoprotein levels of children (19). In an analysis of questionnaire data from 8,765 children aged 5 to
7 years, smoking after pregnancy was not associated with childhood obesity but intrauterine exposure was (20). Another study found that smoking during the 12 months before birth of a child was associated with adolescent overweight (21).
Studies have found a significant association between maternal prepregnancy overweight or obesity and overweight in children. This association indicates that overweight mothers are more likely to have overweight children, and these odds increase with the age of the child. For children aged 24 to 47
months, only maternal prepregnancy obesity had a significant effect; for children aged 48
to 71 months, either maternal prepregnancy overweight or obesity increased risk; and in children aged 72
to 95 months,
maternal overweight or obesity imparted an even higher risk (21,22). Breast-feeding reduced the likelihood of early adolescent overweight in children whose mothers’ prepregnancy BMI was 25 or higher, although the effect of breast-feeding was not significant in children of healthy-weight mothers (21,22). Thus, both prenatal and maternal variables can increase the risk of obesity in even the youngest children, long before social factors have an influence. Obesity prevention efforts at this
stage of development have typically focused on encouraging healthy prenatal nutrition and breast-feeding; however, interventions to reduce maternal obesity during pregnancy have been limited.
Back to top
Infancy and Early Childhood
Taste acquisition and preference
The biological substrate that underlies the taste and rewarding properties of foods is relevant
because the best predictor of food preference is whether a child likes the taste (23). Whether a food tastes good or bad and the pleasure of eating is a complex process mediated by chemical senses in the periphery and multiple brain substrates, which are remarkably well conserved phylogenetically (24).
The degree to which taste and smell are agreeable is determined by innate factors (25), learning and experience, and the interactions among these. From an evolutionary perspective, these senses, which are well developed at birth, function as gatekeepers throughout life (26). The small number of taste qualities may have evolved because of the functional importance of the primary stimuli (eg, sugars, sodium chloride, bitter toxins) in nutrient selection, especially in
children. The heightened preference for sweet taste, which is evident within hours after birth and persists until adolescence (27,28) most likely evolved because sweet-tasting foods are high in energy. Children’s heightened preference for salty tastes (29) attracts them to necessary minerals, and rejecting bitter-tasting substances protects them from poisons
because most poisonous compounds taste bitter (30). However, although bitter tastes are innately disliked, with repeated exposure,
infants can come to like certain foods that are bitter, particularly some vegetables (31-34).
The 2002 Feeding Infants and Toddlers Study, designed to update knowledge on the feeding patterns of the youngest Americans, found that even before their second birthday, many American toddlers develop the unhealthy eating habits of adults (35).
Although toddlers were more likely to eat fruits than vegetables, 1 in 4 did not eat any vegetables on a given day. Instead, like older children, they were more likely to eat fatty foods such as
french fries, salty snacks, and sweet beverages and less
likely to eat bitter-tasting vegetables (36,37). None of the top 5 vegetables eaten by toddlers was a dark green vegetable.
Our knowledge is growing of how, beginning very early in life, early sensory experience can shape and modify flavor and food preferences. For example, fetuses exposed to flavors, usually detected by the sense of smell, in amniotic fluid and infants exposed to flavors in breast milk (both of which reflect flavors of the mother’s diet) (38) learn to like those flavors as they make the transition to eating adult foods (39). The foods that women eat when they are pregnant and
nursing are precisely the ones that their infants should prefer because the mothers’ eating them teaches the child that these foods are available, safe, and nutritious. At this time, however, how the protective factor of breast-feeding interacts with transmitting flavor preference for energy-dense foods in overweight mothers is unclear.
These sensory and biological considerations shed light on why lifestyle changes are difficult for young children to make. We cannot easily change the basic ingrained biology of liking sweets and avoiding bitterness — preferring candy to spinach. What we can do is modulate children’s flavor preferences by providing early exposure, starting in utero and early infancy, to a variety of healthy flavors. The first emotional attachment to flavors should be exploited to try to
reduce the prevalence of obesity in future generations. For this reason, preventive interventions may be most effective during pregnancy and postpartum, when women are highly motivated to change for the benefit of their children. Pregnant and lactating women should widen their food choices to include as many flavorful and healthy foods as possible. These experiences, combined with repeated exposure to nutritious foods and flavor variety (31-34), should make children more likely to choose a
healthy diet.
Eating in the absence of hunger
Infants (40,41) and young children (42,43) can adjust their food intake in response to changes in the caloric content of their diet, and biological sensations involving appetite probably underlie this ability. This ability has been documented at meals (42,43) and during the course of a day (44). The complex interaction of nature and nurture in the regulation of appetite
(45) is exemplified by a behavior known as eating in the absence of hunger (EAH), a behavioral marker of impaired satiety
(46-49). Children ranging in age from 3 (50) to 19 years (51) have been observed in laboratory settings to eat large amounts of palatable food in the absence of hunger, after a meal. The amount of energy consumed in the absence of hunger is variable and related to child weight. EAH is seen more often in children who are overweight (39,47,49,52) and in children with higher 1-year weight gains (53).
This behavior is analogous to external (53) or disinhibited (54) eating behavior
in adults.
Genes influence many aspects of eating behavior, including taste sensitivity
(55), food preference (56), intake of specific foods (57), meal patterns (58),
energy density (59), macronutrient intake (60,61), and meal (61-63) and daily
energy intake (61). Behaviors such as EAH may also be heritable (64), although evidence is limited. Genetic influences on EAH (49,65) and other eating behaviors such as emotional eating (66) are supported by findings that the behavior is more common when 1 or both
parents are overweight, even after certain environmental factors (eg, parental eating habits) have been controlled for. Evidence for genetic influence on intake regulation is also reflected in the relative stability of behaviors like EAH within people during 2- to 6-year periods in childhood (47,67).
The biological underpinnings of EAH and other appetite-related behaviors are not well understood. EAH in children is associated with higher fasting insulin and leptin levels (64), 2 hormones that regulate appetite and body weight (68). Satiety responsiveness, a separate dimension of child intake regulation, has recently been linked to variations in the
FTO gene (69), which confers obesity risk and is highly expressed in the hypothalamus, a center of appetite regulation in the brain.
Similar to other aspects of appetite regulation (70,71), EAH appears to become more problematic throughout childhood (45,72). Though the causes are not known, socioenvironmental influences contribute to developmental shifts in intake regulation by overriding biologically based cues of hunger and satiety. Factors that modify intake regulation include the types and amounts of food to which children are exposed, social modeling of eating behaviors, and child feeding styles and practices (73).
For instance, experimental research has demonstrated social modeling influences on both the types (74) and amounts of food eaten by young children (75-77).
Studies of EAH among girls have shown positive associations with mothers’ but not fathers’ disinhibited eating (52,65).
EAH has also been associated with restrictive feeding practices, although not consistently. Restricting children’s access to a preferred food has been associated with higher levels of EAH in girls aged 3 to 5 years (49,78)
and in non-Hispanic white girls aged 5 to 9 years (47,72,79). Laboratory studies of preschool-aged children have also demonstrated that restrictions placed on children’s access to palatable, energy-dense foods can lead to increased food intake when restrictions
are lifted and food becomes available (80,81). Other studies, however, found no link between feeding restriction and EAH (54).
In many ways, inconsistencies in the literature on EAH parallel those observed in the general literature on child feeding, which may reflect the early stage of the work in the field. Knowledge of child feeding has largely evolved from laboratory studies that address cause and effect but provide
limited insight on the usual environments and social interactions surrounding children’s behavior. The approach parents take to feeding their children reflects their goals for their children’s eating and health, and these goals are influenced by culture and socioeconomic status (82). To some extent, the effect of child feeding practices on children’s health requires careful consideration of context.
Back to top
Middle Childhood
Another critical period for the development of obesity is during middle childhood. BMI tends to decrease during early childhood and then, typically between the ages of 6 and 8, begins to rise again (adiposity rebound). Excessive rebound and early rebound (before age 5) are related to higher BMI in adulthood (83). An early rebound may reflect the child’s taking more control of intake, exposure to
gestational diabetes, or early maturation (84).
Children’s patterns of weight gain vary by sex and age (85), and during stages of rapid growth, caloric requirements increase. These stages are opportunities for interventions to prevent obesity by controlling caloric intake and increasing energy expenditure. In addition, because the prevalence of obesity increases
among children after puberty, as the age of sexual maturity decreases in the population (86) obesity will probably become more prevalent among elementary school students.
Children of school age are highly susceptible to environmental stimuli such as marketing and food availability.
Studies suggest that children who are exposed to food advertisements eat more. Several studies of advertising on children’s television programs found that the foods promoted increased
the risk of becoming obese. In
1 study, at least half of food advertisements during children’s television programming were for energy-dense, low-nutrient foods such as
cereal, candy, snacks, soda, and fast food (87). Not only do such advertisements promote eating, but eating while watching television
also often leads to overeating because children do not notice how much they are eating (88). A recent study showed that for each hour of television watched, children consumed an extra 167 kcal/d (89). This susceptibility is mainly because decision making, critical thinking, and abstract thinking are underdeveloped in childhood. For example,
children in Piaget’s
preoperational stage of cognitive development (ages 2-7) are characterized by illogical and egocentric thought, while children in the concrete operational stage (ages 7-11) cannot think abstractly, reason logically, or make inferences based on available information (90).
Exposure to advertisements would decrease by restricting food advertisements that target children, especially during times,
such as Saturday mornings, when many children are watching television (87).
The school environment is an opportunity for study and intervention in children’s health behaviors because multiple factors can influence obesity in this context. For example, the Coordinated School Health Program model from the Centers for Disease Control and Prevention proposes a multilevel approach in which 8 different school components (eg, health education, nutrition services, healthy school environment, family and community involvement) interact to influence student health (91).
Few studies of school eating patterns have focused on kindergarten and early elementary years, but in third grade, school lunch choices begin to influence children’s overall diets (92). Schools’ food policies affect student BMI; in 1 study, as the number of school food policies increased, students’ mean BMI decreased (93).
The US educational system has typically fallen short in considering health a priority for academic emphasis or outcomes. Health outcomes must be included in the educational agenda and become part of school accountability to obtain support and funding for health-based policies and interventions, such as physical education, comprehensive health education, or BMI monitoring. A survey of the 100 largest school districts in 2006 found that, among the local wellness policies implemented,
99% dealt with nutrition standards of school meals, 97% required nutrition education for at least some grades, and 65% set standards for when teachers can use food to reward children for good behavior or academic accomplishments
(94). School programs intended to alleviate the obesity crisis need funding, partnerships, and evaluation. In 2007, the Registered Nurses’ Association of Ontario set forth school policies that would prevent childhood obesity; these policies included promoting
physical education classes for all students, requiring physical education specialists to be involved in physical education classes, selling healthy foods in cafeterias and vending machines, and promoting walking or biking to and from school (95).
Back to top
Adolescence
In addition to developmental risks carried from earlier life, by adolescence the cumulative effects of social disadvantage on obesity become apparent. Analysis of data from the National Longitudinal Survey of Youth Child-Mother File found that having an unmarried mother increased the risk for adolescent overweight. Education and current income were not significantly associated with adolescent overweight, and lifetime income was only marginally significant (21).
The Growing Up Today Study found that subjective social status in the school environment predicted BMI in adolescent girls (96). Girls who ranked themselves at the low end of school social status were 69% more likely to have
a BMI 2 kg/m2 higher than that of girls of higher subjective social status. The authors concluded that higher subjective social standing in school might protect against weight gain in adolescent girls. The feedback loop in which low self-esteem increases the risk of
overweight and overweight contributes to low self-esteem could be a critical point of intervention.
An analysis of data from 12,067 people in the Framingham Heart Study revealed an association between people’s weight gain and weight gain in their social networks (97). Rather than occurring randomly throughout social networks, people with BMI
≥30 kg/m2 were clustered in the 32-year data set. This finding was not explained solely by social ties between people who were already obese. The chance of becoming obese during a given period increased with development of
obesity in a friend (57% increase), a sibling (40% increase), or a spouse (37% increase). The effect was stronger between same-sex friends and siblings. This association did not extend to neighbors, nor was it associated with changes in smoking behavior. A more recent study examined the peer effects on adolescent BMI by using the National Longitudinal Study of Adolescent Health (98). This study found an effect of social networks on obesity in adolescents, and the effect was more pronounced
among girls and heavier adolescents. Social networks seem to play a role in the spread of obesity in both adults and adolescents. Therefore, programs that target peer norms may be effective in preventing overweight in adolescents.
Cognitive development during adolescence should also be considered.
Although the developmental stage of formal operational thought enables skills such as enhanced problem solving, decision making, and abstract reasoning (90), the underdeveloped prefrontal cortex (99) still leaves adolescents at risk for behaviors that may increase the risk of obesity. For example, adolescents tend to be prone to impulsivity
and the fallacy of invincibility and to have problems considering long-term consequences
of their behavior. These factors can contribute to poor judgment when it comes to food selection and other health-related behaviors.
Back to top
Conclusions
Traditional interventions for child and adolescent obesity often focus on the individual child, with or without family involvement, and include education, modification of diet, and increases in physical activity. Cognitive behavioral strategies are often used to help children make better decisions, solve problems, and monitor their own progress. A recent movement suggests minimizing screen time for youth because television, computers, and video games contribute to sedentary behavior.
However, few obesity interventions show clinically or statistically significant weight loss beyond the intervention period (100), which suggests that
new and more comprehensive interventions are needed.
To counteract the growing incidence of obesity, interventions must adopt an approach that grasps the interplay of economic, social, behavioral, biomedical, and environmental influences. Such an approach would have to encompass emerging knowledge about how obesity is the consequence of complex factors acting at many stages throughout a person’s lifetime. The challenge of intervening in the obesity epidemic becomes even more daunting with the realization that, in children and
adolescents, these influences must be considered separately at each stage of development.
This overview of the developmental influences on childhood overweight and obesity suggests opportunities for intervention. To combat prenatal influences on child obesity, pregnant women should be strongly discouraged from smoking and encouraged to consume a diet low in fat with adequate protein. Additionally, breast-feeding may decrease the risk of overweight, particularly in children born to overweight mothers. Because women are more motivated to change behaviors during pregnancy and
immediately postpartum, these intervals can be targeted to shape eating patterns of both mother and child.
Interventions to prevent child and adolescent obesity should focus on multiple settings, including the home and school. Further research should investigate variables in family relationships, the home, and the extended environment that influence eating. Schools can intervene by offering healthy food choices in their breakfast and lunch programs and vending machines. Empowering families and schools and giving them resources to engage in obesity prevention efforts and
to provide environments that support
healthy behaviors are critical issues that governments and social institutions need to address.
Back to top
Author Information
Corresponding Author: Terry T. Huang, PhD, MPH, Eunice Kennedy Shriver National Institute of Child Health and Human Development, 6100 Executive Blvd, 4B11, Bethesda, MD 20892-7510. Telephone: 301-594-1846. E-mail:
huangter@mail.nih.gov.
Author Affiliations: Layla Esposito, Eunice Kennedy Shriver National Institute of Child Health and Human Development, Bethesda, Maryland; Jennifer O. Fisher, Temple University, Philadelphia, Pennsylvania; Julie A. Mennella, Monell Chemical Senses Center, Philadelphia, Pennsylvania; Deanna M. Hoelscher, University of Texas School of Public Health, Houston, Texas.
Back to top
References
- Oken E, Gillman MW.
Fetal origins of obesity. Obes Res 2003;11(4):496-506.
- Barker DJ, Eriksson JG, Forsen T, Osmond C.
Fetal origins of adult disease: strength of effects and biological basis. Int J Epidemiol 2002;31:1235-9.
- Huang TT, Glass T.
Transforming research strategies for understanding and preventing obesity. JAMA 2008;300(15):1811-3.
- Glass TA, McAtt MJ.
Behavioral science at the crossroads in public health: extending horizons, envisioning the future. Soc Sci Med 2006;62(7):1650-71.
- Huang TT, Drewnowski A, Kumanyika SK, Glass TA. A systems-oriented framework for tackling obesity in the 21st century. Prev Chronic Dis 2009;6(3).
http://www.cdc.gov/pcd/issues/2009/jul/09_0013.htm.
- Drake AJ, Walker BR, Seckl JR.
Intergenerational consequences of fetal programming by in utero exposure to glucocorticoids in rats. Am J Physiol Regul Integr Comp Physiol 2005;288:R34-8.
- McMillen IC, Robinson JS.
Developmental origins of the metabolic syndrome: prediction, plasticity, and programming. Physiol Rev 2005;85(2):571-633.
- Langley SC, Jackson AA.
Increased systolic blood pressure in adult rats induced by fetal exposure to maternal low protein diets. Clin Sci (Lond) 1994;86(2):217-22.
- Brawley L, Itoh S, Torrens C, Barker A, Bertram C, Hanson P, et al.
Dietary protein restriction in pregnancy induces hypertension and vascular defects in rat male offspring. Pediatr Res 2003;54(1):83-90.
- Berney DM, Desai M, Palmer DJ, Greenwald S, Brown A, Hales CN, et al.
The effects of maternal protein deprivation on the fetal rat pancreas: major structural changes and their recuperation. J Pathol 1997;183(1):109-15.
- Langley SC, Browne RF, Jackson AA.
Altered glucose tolerance in rats exposed to maternal low protein diets in utero. Comp Biochem Physiol Physiol 1994;109(2):223-9.
- Burns SP, Desai M, Cohen RD, Hales CN, Iles RA, Germain JP, et al.
Gluconeogenesis, glucose handling, and structural changes in livers of the adult offspring of rats partially deprived of protein during pregnancy and lactation. J Clin Invest 1997;100(7):1768-74.
- Sparre T, Reusens B, Cherif H, Larsen MR, Roepstorff P, Fey SJ, et al.
Intrauterine programming of fetal islet gene expression in rats — effects of maternal protein restriction during gestation revealed by proteome analysis. Diabetologia 2003;46(11):1497-511.
- Godfrey K, Robinson S, Barker DJ, Osmond C, Cox V.
Maternal nutrition in early and late pregnancy in relation to placental and fetal growth. BMJ 1996;312(7028):410-4.
- Khan IY, Dekou V, Douglas G, Jensen R, Hanson MA, Poston L, et al.
A high-fat diet during rat pregnancy or suckling induces cardiovascular dysfunction in adult offspring. Am J Physiol Regul Integr Comp Physiol 2005;288:R127-33.
- Dubois L, Girard M.
Early determinants of overweight at 4.5 years in a population-based longitudinal study. Int J Obes (Lond) 2006;30:610-7.
- Oken E, Levitan EB, Gilman MW.
Maternal smoking during pregnancy and child overweight: systematic review and meta-analysis. Int J Obes (Lond) 2008;32:201-10.
- Power C, Jefferis BJ.
Fetal environment and subsequent obesity: a study of maternal smoking. Int J Epidemiol 2002;31(2):413-9.
- Jaddoe VW, de Ridder MA, van den Elzen AP, Hofman A, Uiterwaal CS, Witteman JC.
Maternal smoking in pregnancy is associated with cholesterol development in the offspring: a 27-year follow-up study. Atherosclerosis 2008;196(1):42-8.
- Toschke AM, Koletzko B, Slikker W Jr, Hermann M, von Kries R.
Childhood obesity is associated with maternal smoking in pregnancy. Eur J Pediatr 2002;161(8):445-8.
- Salsberry PJ, Reagan PB.
Taking the long view: the prenatal environment and early adolescent overweight. Res Nurs Health 2007;30(3):297-307.
- Salsberry PJ, Reagan PB.
Dynamics of early childhood overweight. Pediatrics 2005;116(6):1329-38.
- Resnicow K, Davis-Hearn M, Smith M, Baranowski T, Lin LS, Baranowski J, et al.
Social-cognitive predictors of fruit and vegetable intake in children. Health Psychol 1997;16:272-6.
- Berridge K, Kringelbach ML.
Affective neuroscience of pleasure: reward in humans and animals. Psychopharmacology 2008;199:457-80.
- Mennella JA, Pepino MY, Reed DR.
Genetic and environmental determinants of bitter perception and sweet preferences. Pediatrics 2005;115(2):e216-22.
- Ganchrow JR, Mennella JA. The ontogeny of human flavor perception. In: Doty RL, editor. Handbook of olfaction and gestation. 2nd edition. New York (NY): Marcel Dekker, Inc; 2003. p. 823-46.
- Maller O, Turner RE.
Taste in acceptance of sugars by human infants. J Comp Physiol Psychol 1973;84:496-501.
- Desor JA, Beauchamp GK.
Longitudinal changes in sweet preferences in humans. Physiol Behav 1987;39:639-41.
- Beauchamp GK, Cowart BJ, Moran M.
Developmental changes in salt acceptability in human infants. Dev Psychobiol 1986;19:17-25.
- Glendinning JI.
Is the bitter rejection response always adaptive? Physiol Behav 1994;56:1217-27.
- Forestell CA, Mennella JA.
Early determinants of fruit and vegetable acceptance. Pediatrics 2007;120:1247-54.
- Gerrish CJ, Mennella JA.
Flavor variety enhances food acceptance in formula-fed infants. Am J Clin Nutr 2001;73:1080-5.
- Mennella JA, Nicklaus S, Jagolina AL, Yourshaw LM.
Variety is the spice of life: strategies for promoting fruit and vegetable acceptance during infancy. Physiol Behav 2008;94:29-38.
- Sullivan SA, Birch LL.
Infant dietary experience and acceptance of solid foods. Pediatrics 1994;93:271-7.
- Ziegler P, Briefel R, Clusen N, Devaney B.
Feeding Infants and Toddlers Study (FITS): development of the FITS survey in comparison to other dietary survey methods. J Am Diet Assoc 2006;106(1 Suppl 1):S12-27.
- Mennella JA, Kennedy JM, Beauchamp GK.
Vegetable acceptance by infants: effects of formula flavors. Early Hum Dev 2006;82(7):463-8.
- Fox MK, Pac S, Devaney B, Jankowski L.
Feeding Infants and Toddlers Study: what foods are infants and toddlers eating? J Am Diet Assoc 2004;104(Suppl 1):S22-30.
- Mennella JA, Griffin CE, Beauchamp GK.
Flavor programming during infancy. Pediatrics 2004;113(4):840-5.
- Mennella JA, Jagnow CP, Beauchamp GK.
Prenatal and postnatal flavor learning by human infants. Pediatrics 2001;107(6):E88.
- Fomon SJ, Filer LJ Jr, Thomas LN, Rogers RR, Proksch AM.
Relationship between formula concentration and rate of growth of normal infants. J Nutr 1969;98(2):241-54.
- Fomon SJ, Filmer LJ Jr, Thomas LN, Anderson TA, Nelson SE.
Influence of formula concentration on caloric intake and growth of normal infants. Acta Paediatr Scand 1975;64(2):172-81.
- Birch LL, Deysher M. Conditioned and unconditioned caloric compensation: evidence for self-regulation of food intake by young children. Learn Motiv 1985;16:341-55.
- Birch LL, Deysher M.
Caloric compensation and sensory specific satiety: evidence for self-regulation of food intake by young children. Appetite 1986;7(4):323-31.
- Birch LL, Johnson SL, Jones MB, Peters JC.
Effects of a nonenergy fat substitute on children’s energy and macronutrient intake. Am J Clin Nutr 1993;58(3):326-33.
- Blundell JE, Lawton CL, Cotton JR, Macdiarmid JI.
Control of human appetite: implications for the intake of dietary fat. Annu Rev Nutr 1996;16:285-319.
- Hill C, Llewellyn CH, Saxton J, Webber L, Semmler C, Carnell S, et al.
Adiposity and “eating in the absence of hunger” in children. Int J Obes (Lond) 2008;32(10):1499-505.
- Fisher JO, Birch LL.
Eating in the absence of hunger and overweight in girls from 5 to 7 y of age. Am J Clin Nutr 2002;76(1):226-31.
- Fisher JO, Butte NF, Jaramillo S. Eating in the absence of hunger as a behavioral phenotype of overweight Hispanic children. Obes Res 2003;11:A97.
- Faith MS, Berkowitz RI, Stallings VA, Kerns J, Storey M, Stunkard AJ.
Eating in the absence of hunger: a genetic marker for childhood obesity in prepubertal boys? Obesity (Silver Spring) 2006;14(1):131-8.
- Cutting TM, Fisher JO, Grimm-Thomas K, Birch LL.
Like mother, like daughter: familial patterns of overweight are mediated by mothers’ dietary disinhibition. Am J Clin Nutr 1999;69(4):608-13.
- Butte NF, Cai G, Cole SA, Wilson T, Fisher J, Zakeri I, et al.
Metabolic and behavioral predictors of weight gain in Hispanic children: the Viva la Familia Study. Am J Clin Nutr 2007;85(6):1478-85.
- Moens E, Braet C.
Predictors of disinhibited eating in children with and without overweight. Behav Res Ther 2007;45(6):1357-68.
- Van Strien T, Fritjers JER, Bergers GPA, Defares PB. The Dutch Eating Behavior Questionnaire (DEBQ) for assessment of restrained, emotional, and external behavior. Int J Eat Disord 1986;5(2):295-315.
- Stunkard AJ, Messick S.
The 3-factor eating questionnaire to measure dietary restraint, disinhibition and hunger. J Psychosom Res 1985;29(1):71-83.
- Mennella JA, Pepino MY, Reed DR.
Genetic and environmental determinants of bitter perception and sweet preferences. Pediatrics 2005;115:e216-22.
- Falciglia GA, Norton PA.
Evidence for a genetic influence on preference for some foods. J Am Diet Assoc 1994;94(2):154-8.
- de Castro JM.
A twin study of genetic and environmental influences on the intake of fluids and beverages. Physiol Behav 1993;54(4):677-87.
- Keski-Rahkonen A, Viken RJ, Kaprio J, Rissanen A, Rose RJ.
Genetic and environmental factors in breakfast eating patterns. Behav Genet 2004;34(5):503-14.
- de Castro JM.
Heredity influences the dietary energy density of free-living humans. Physiol Behav 2006;87(1):192-8.
- Wade J, Milner J, Krondl M.
Evidence for a physiological regulation of food selection and nutrient intake in twins. Am J Clin Nutr 1981;34(2):143-7.
- de Castro JM.
Independence of genetic influences on body size, daily intake, and meal patterns of humans. Physiol Behav 1993;54(4):633-9.
- Faith MS, Rha SS, Neale MC, Allison DB.
Evidence for genetic influences on human energy intake: results from a twin study using measured observations. Behav Genet 1999;29(3):145-54.
- Faith MS, Keller KL, Johnson SL, Pietrobelli A, Matz P, Must S, et al.
Familial aggregation of energy intake in children. Am J Clin Nutr 2004;79(5):844-50.
- Fisher JO, Cai G, Jaramillo SJ, Cole SA, Comuzzie AG, Butte NF.
Heritability of hyperphagic eating behavior and appetite-related hormones among Hispanic children. Obesity 2007;15(6):1484-95.
- Francis LA, Ventura AK, Marini M, Birch LL.
Parent overweight predicts daughters’ increase in BMI and disinhibited overeating from 5 to 13 years. Obesity 2007;15(6):1544-53.
- Jahnke DL, Warschburger PA.
Familial transmission of eating behaviors in preschool-aged children. Obesity (Silver Spring) 2008;16(8):1821-5.
- Ashcroft J, Semmler C, Carnell S, van Jaarsveld CH, Wardle J.
Continuity and stability of eating behaviour traits in children. Eur J Clin Nutr 2008;62(8):985-90.
- Woods SC, D’Alessio DA.
Central control of body weight and appetite. J Clin Endocrinol Metab 2008;93(11 Suppl 1):S37-50.
- Wardle J, Carnell S, Haworth CM, Farooqi IS, O’Rahilly S, Plomin R.
Obesity associated genetic variation in
FTO is associated with diminished satiety. J Clin Endocrinol Metab 2008;93(9):3640-3.
- Johnson SL, Taylor-Holloway LA.
Non-Hispanic white and Hispanic elementary school children’s self-regulation of energy intake. Am J Clin Nutr 2006;83(6):1276-82.
- Cecil JE, Palmer CN, Wrieden W, Murrie I, Bolton-Smith C, Watt P, et al.
Energy intakes of children after preloads: adjustment, not compensation. Am J Clin Nutr 2005;82(2):302-8.
- Birch LL, Fisher JO, Davison KK.
Learning to overeat: maternal use of restrictive feeding practices promotes girls’ eating in the absence of hunger. Am J Clin Nutr 2003;78(2):215-20.
- Savage JS, Fisher JO, Birch LL.
Parental influence on eating behavior: conception to adolescence. J Law Med Ethics 2007;35(1):22-34.
- Birch LL. Effects of peer models’ food choices and eating behaviors on preschoolers’ food preferences. Child Dev 1980;51:489-96.
- Lumeng JC, Hillman KH.
Eating in larger groups increases food consumption. Arch Dis Child 2007;92(5):384-7.
- Salvy SJ, Coelho JS, Kieffer E, Epstein LH.
Effects of social contexts on overweight and normal-weight children’s food intake. Physiol Behav 2007;92(5):840-6.
- Salvy SJ, Vartanian LR, Coelho JS, Jarrin D, Pliner PP.
The role of familiarity on modeling of eating and food consumption in children. Appetite 2008;50(2-3):514-8.
- Fisher JO, Birch LL.
Restricting access to foods and children’s eating. Appetite 1999;32(3):405-19.
- Birch LL, Fisher JO.
Mothers’ child feeding practices influences daughters’ eating and weight. Am J Clin Nutr 2000;71:1054-61.
- Fisher JO, Birch LL.
Restricting access to palatable foods affects children’s behavioral response, food selection, and intake. Am J Clin Nutr 1999;69:1264-72.
- Jansen E, Mulkens S, Jansen A.
Do not eat the red food!: prohibition of snacks leads to their relatively higher consumption in children. Appetite 2007;49(3):572-7.
- LeVine RA. Human parental care: universal goals, cultural strategies, individual behavior. In: Levine RA, Miller PM, West MM, editors. Parental behavior in diverse societies. San Francisco (CA): Jossey-Bass; 1988. p. 3-12.
- Whitaker RC, Pepe MS, Wright, JA, Seidel KD, Dietz W.
Early adiposity rebound and the risk of adult obesity. Pediatrics 1998;101(3):E5.
- Dietz WH. Periods of risk in childhood for the development of adult obesity — what do we need to learn? J Nutr 1997;127:1884S-6S.
- Wang Y. Is obesity associated with early sexual maturation? A comparison of the association in American boys versus girls. Pediatrics 2002;110:903-10.
- Himes J. Examining the evidence for recent secular changes in the timing of puberty in US children in light of increases in the prevalence of obesity. Mol Cell Endocrinol 2006;254-255:13-21.
- McGinnis JM, Gottman JA, Kraak VI, editors. Food marketing to children and youth: threat or opportunity? Washington (DC): National Academies Press; 2006.
- French SA, Story M, Jeffery RW.
Environmental influences on eating and physical activity. Annu Rev Public Health 2001;22:309-35.
- Wiecha JL, Peterson KE, Ludwig DS, Kim J, Sobol A, Gortmaker SL.
When children eat what they watch: impact of television viewing on dietary intake in youth. Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med 2006;160:436-42.
- Piaget J. Piaget’s theory. In: Mussen P, editor. Handbook of child psychology. 4th edition. New York (NY): Wiley; 1983. p. 103-28.
- Allensworth DD, Kolbe LJ.
The comprehensive school health program: exploring an expanded concept. J Sch Health 1987;57(10):409-12.
- Lytle LA, Stone EJ, Nichaman MZ, Perry CL, Montgomery DH, Nicklas TA, et al.
Changes in nutrient intakes of elementary school children following a school-based intervention: results from the CATCH study. Prev Med 1996;25(4):465-77.
- Kubik MY, Lytle LA, Story M.
Schoolwide food practices are associated with body mass index in middle school students. Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med 2005;159(12):1111-4.
- Moag-Stahlberg A, Howley N, Luscri L.
A national snapshot of local school wellness policies. J Sch Health 2008;78:562-8.
- School Nutrition Association 2006-07 annual report: touching lives through wellness. http://docs.schoolnutrition.org/files/SNAannualRPT07.pdf. Accessed March 25, 2008.
- Lemeshow AR, Fisher L, Goodman E, Kawachi I, Berkey CS, Colditz GA.
Subjective social status in the school and change in adiposity in female adolescents: findings from a prospective cohort study. Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med 2008;162(1):23-8.
- Christakis NA, Fowler JH.
The spread of obesity in a large social network over 32 years. N Engl J Med 2007;357(4):370-9.
- Trogdon JG, Nonnemaker J, Pais J.
Peer effects in adolescent overweight. J Health Econ 2008;27:1388-99.
- Thompson PM, Giedd JN, Woods RP, MacDonald D, Evans AC, Toga AW, et al.
Growth patterns in the developing brain detected by using continuum mechanical tensor maps. Nature 2000;404(6774):190-3.
- Summerbell CD, Waters E, Edmunds LD, Kelly S, Brown T, Campbell KJ.
Interventions for preventing obesity in children. Cochrane Database Syst Rev 2005;(3):CD001871.
Back to top
|
|