Relationship of Age for Grade and Pubertal Stage to Early Initiation of Substance Use
ORIGINAL RESEARCH — Volume 12 — November 19, 2015
Rebecca N. Dudovitz, MD, MS; Paul J. Chung, MD, MS; Marc N. Elliott, PhD; Susan L. Davies, PhD; Susan Tortolero, PhD; Elizabeth Baumler, PhD; Stephen W. Banspach, PhD; Mark A. Schuster, MD, PhD
Suggested citation for this article: Dudovitz RN, Chung PJ, Elliott MN, Davies SL, Tortolero S, Baumler E, et al. Relationship of Age for Grade and Pubertal Stage to Early Initiation of Substance Use. Prev Chronic Dis 2015;12:150234. DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.5888/pcd12.150234.
PEER REVIEWED
PEER REVIEWED
Abstract
Introduction
Studies suggest students who are substantially older than the average age for their grade engage in risky health behaviors, including substance use. However, most studies do not account for the distinct reasons why students are old for their grade (ie, grade retention vs delayed school entry) or for their pubertal stage. Thus, whether the association between age for grade and substance use is confounded by these factors is unknown. We sought to determine whether age, grade, or pubertal stage were associated with early substance use.
Methods
Cross-sectional Healthy Passages Wave I survey data from 5,147 fifth graders and their caregivers in Alabama, California, and Texas from 2004 through 2006 were analyzed in 2014. Logistic regressions examined whether older age for grade, grade retention, delayed school entry, or pubertal stage were associated with use of any substance, cigarettes, alcohol, or other drugs.
Results
Seventeen percent of fifth graders reported trying at least 1 substance. Among boys, advanced pubertal stage was associated with increased odds of cigarette, alcohol, or other drug use, whereas delayed school entry was associated with lower odds of any substance, alcohol, or other drug use. Among girls, advanced pubertal stage was associated only with higher odds of alcohol use, and delayed school entry was not associated with substance use. Neither older age for grade or grade retention was independently associated with substance use after controlling for potential confounders.
Conclusion
Advanced pubertal stage may be a more important risk factor for substance use than age for grade. Pediatricians should consider initiating substance use screening earlier for patients with advanced pubertal stage.
Introduction
In any classroom, a substantial proportion of students are significantly older than their same-grade peers, that is, they are old for grade (OFG) (1). Although some students are OFG because they repeated a grade (grade retention), others are OFG because they started school later, often because of intentionally delayed entry into elementary school (delayed school entry). Becoming OFG by grade retention or delayed school entry occurs relatively early in childhood. For instance, Byrd et al found that among OFG 17-year-old students, 77% had become OFG by third grade (2). Additionally, the number of children with delayed school entry increased during the past 40 years. A 2005 study estimated that 15% of 6-year-old children had not yet started first grade (3). This trend toward “academic redshirting” may be driven by research suggesting that students who start school older than their peers have a modest but significant educational advantage (3–5). Although decisions on school enrollment and advancement are made primarily for educational reasons (6), they may have implications for a child’s mental and behavioral health.
Numerous studies have found higher rates of high-risk behaviors, including substance use, sexual risk-taking, school infractions, and gun-carrying, among adolescent OFG students (7–11). However, in these studies, all OFG students are pooled, without differentiation by their reason for being OFG, so it is not known whether the association between being OFG and high-risk behaviors is found in grade-retained OFG students only, delayed school-entry OFG students only, or both. Given evidence that poor academic performance is associated with high-risk behaviors, grade-retained students may be more likely at baseline to adopt unhealthy behaviors (5,12). Likewise, because behavioral or academic readiness concerns might motivate delayed school entry, redshirted students may also be more likely at baseline to adopt unhealthy behaviors despite their slightly better average academic performance.
Other factors may confound the relationship between OFG and risky health behaviors. For instance, the experience of being older might place OFG students at higher risk for unhealthy behaviors than their peers because of their relatively advanced physical and social–emotional maturation, particularly with respect to the onset of puberty. Although they did not control for pubertal stage or timing, Byrd et al found that being OFG, even without grade retention, was associated with higher rates of behavior problems compared with students who were neither OFG nor grade retained (2).
In general, the association between delayed school entry and health behaviors is poorly understood. Some studies suggest that delayed school entry is a risk factor for poor academic performance, particularly among socially disadvantaged children (1) and might be associated with high-risk behaviors (2,7–9,11); however, other studies suggest that delayed school entry might lead to — or at least be a marker of factors that lead to — better academic achievement and less risky health behaviors (3–5,13). For example, parents who choose to postpone enrolling their child in school may have more financial resources (allowing an additional year of care at home or in preschool) or be more engaged in their child’s education (13). Finally, although both the reasons for being OFG and the experience of being OFG might differ between boys and girls (2), no study examines whether associations between OFG and substance use differ by sex.
Determining whether OFG is independently associated with risky health behaviors, or whether it is merely a marker of other contextual factors driving this association, is important for accurate counseling of families on school entry and advancement and for developing school policies that maximize population health. This study examined data from a preadolescent population (1) to determine the effects of age for grade and pubertal stage and to increase our understanding of the relationships among OFG, delayed school entry, grade retention, and substance use.
Methods
In 2014, we analyzed data from Wave I (2004–2006) of Healthy Passages, a survey of 5,147 fifth graders and their primary caregivers from Los Angeles, California; Birmingham, Alabama; and Houston, Texas. At each site, we used a 2-stage probability sampling procedure in which study schools were sampled from all public schools in the geographic region with enrollment of 25 or more fifth graders. Letters were sent home with 11,532 fifth-grade students from study schools requesting permission to contact each student’s primary caregiver. Among the 6,663 students whose parents permitted the study team to contact them, both the student and one of their primary caregivers were invited to participate. Of those, 5,147 (77%) students completed the interview (14,15).
Data were collected via computer-assisted personal interviews and audio-computer–assisted self-interviews. Each child and 1 primary caregiver were interviewed separately to ensure confidentiality. The study protocol was approved by institutional review boards at each study site and at the Centers for Disease Control and Prevention.
Measures
Predictors
Age for grade was calculated from each student’s birth date, as reported by the primary caregiver. To determine the association between being OFG and substance use, age for grade relative to a student’s immediate same-grade peers was measured. Thus, each student’s age was centered on the mean age for grade at his or her school after excluding 19 influential outliers (deviations of more than 2 years in schools with 35 or fewer observations). A sensitivity analysis that restored the outliers (0.4% of the data) yielded similar results.
Grade retention was assessed by student self-report. Students who indicated that they had ever repeated a grade in school were considered grade-retained.
Delayed school entry was measured indirectly by assuming that students who were older than their expected age for grade and did not have a history of grade retention entered school late. The expected age for grade was calculated from each student’s birth date and the applicable cutoff date for school entry (September 1 for Birmingham and Houston; December 1 for Los Angeles). Students who did not report repeating a grade in school but were in a lower grade than would be expected if they had started school on time and had progressed normally were considered to have delayed school entry.
Initiation of substance use has been associated with the onset of puberty (16), and pubertal stage is likely to be more advanced in OFG students. Hence pubertal stage has the potential to confound a possible association between age for grade and substance use, particularly among early adolescents. Pubertal status was measured using 2 questions that included Tanner staging pictures (pubic hair and genital development for boys; pubic hair and breast development for girls). Students selected the pictures that most closely depicted their stage of physical development, and their 2 responses were averaged to measure overall pubertal status (stage 1–5: 1 = pre-pubescent, 5 = fully mature). Correlation between the items was 0.30 for boys and 0.41 for girls, as expected given the somewhat weak correlation between the timings of androgenic hair development and sexual maturation (17). Pubertal stage varies significantly at this age (18), as indicated by a correlation of 0.24 between chronological age and Tanner self-staging.
Outcomes
One dichotomous (yes/no) item assessed lifetime cigarette smoking by asking students whether they had ever tried smoking cigarettes. One dichotomous item assessed past-year alcohol use by asking students whether they had taken more than a few sips of alcohol in the past 12 months, excluding drinking a few sips of alcohol for religious purposes. A dichotomous measure of whether the child had ever used another drug was derived from student responses to whether they had ever used marijuana, ever sniffed glue or used inhalants to get high, or ever used any pill or other drug to get high. Students who answered yes to any of these questions were considered to have a history of other drug use. Finally, answering yes to any of the above substance use items (cigarette, alcohol, or other drug use) was used as a dichotomous measure of overall substance use initiation.
Covariates
Covariates with potential associations with both age for grade and health behaviors were included.
School functioning
School functioning is likely associated with grade retention, delayed school entry, and substance use, making it an important potential confounder. School functioning was measured via child responses to the PedsQL School Functioning Subscale (19). This 5-item self-report subscale asks the frequency of having problems with paying attention in school, doing school work, forgetting things, missing school because of not feeling well, and missing school because of doctor visits or being in the hospital, with higher scores corresponding to better school functioning. Scores on each item range from 0 to 4 with higher scores reflecting better school function. Scores were summed and multiplied by 5 for an overall range of 0 to 100. In this sample, the subscale had an internal consistency of 0.65 and a mean of 74.1. Additionally, higher school functioning scores were associated with lower odds of having been grade-retained (odds ratio [OR] = 0.747 per 10 points, P < .001), supporting the construct validity of this measure.
The remaining covariates were taken from the parent survey.
School mobility
School mobility was measured by the number of elementary schools attended by the child. The potential effect of changing schools on risk (eg, delinquency, other problem behavior) (20) and protective (eg, school achievement, coping) (21) behaviors in children has been the focus of several studies, although results are inconsistent (22).
Parental involvement
Parental involvement was measured by combining 10 items from the FACES III scale with measures of parental involvement in students’ social and scholastic lives (2, 23). Items were how often the parent attended school events, volunteered at school, ate with the child, did fun things with the child, knew what the child did after school, knew what the child did during free time, knew the child’s friends, knew who the child’s best friend was, and knew the parents of the child’s friends. In this sample, the internal consistency of this measure was 0.80.
Sociodemographics
Data on highest household level of education and household income were used to measure socioeconomic status. Categories of missing values for annual household income and parental level of education were included because these data tend not to be missing at random (24). Additional covariates were family composition (2-parent family, single-parent family, or other), race/ethnicity, study site, interview date, whether the child had health insurance, and whether the child was born in the United States.
Data analysis
Multivariate logistic regressions assessed whether age for grade, grade retention, delayed school entry, or pubertal stage were significantly associated with each substance use outcome.. To isolate the association between chronological age and early initiation of substance use after controlling for potential confounders, we included the linear measure of relative age for grade, an indicator for grade retention, an indicator for delayed school entry, and a linear measure of pubertal stage in the models. Collinearity among the major predictors was low, with variance inflation factor values less than 2 for all predictors of interest. In addition, these multivariate models controlled for school functioning, school mobility, parental involvement, and other sociodemographic covariates.. All analyses were performed separately for boys and girls. Finally, to determine whether delayed school-entry OFG students differed from other students with respect to contextual factors associated with substance use, a multivariate logistic regression of delayed school entry on the covariates in our model was performed. We used Stata software, version 13 (StataCorp) to account for design and nonresponse weights, clustering of children within schools, and site stratification (12).
Results
The mean age of the sample was 10.6 years, ranging from 9.6 to 13.8 years (Table 1). For students who reported grade retention, the mean age was 11.5 years, whereas the mean age for students with delayed school entry was 11.3 years. Overall, 18% of the sample was OFG — 13% reported grade retention, and 5% delayed school entry.
Of the sample, 51.1% were male, although boys constituted 58.9% of grade-retained students and 55.6% of delayed school-entry students. Students from households earning less than $25,000 annually made up 38.1% of the sample, and 44.5% of parents did not attend college. Overall, 17.4% of students reported trying at least one substance: 6.6% reported smoking cigarettes, 5.5% drinking alcohol, and 9.8% using any other drug.
Results from the multivariate logistic regressions including age for grade, grade retention, delayed school entry, pubertal stage, and covariates are shown by sex (Table 2). Among boys, more advanced pubertal stage was associated with higher odds of using any substance (OR = 1.24; 95% confidence interval [CI], 1.05–1.46; P = .01), using cigarettes (OR = 1.55; 95% CI, 1.23–1.95; P < .001), and using alcohol (OR = 1.32; 95% CI, 1.08–1.62; P = .007). Neither age for grade or grade retention was significantly associated with substance use. However, delayed school entry was associated with lower odds of initiating use of any substance (OR = 0.31; 95% CI, 0.16–0.60; P = .001), use of alcohol (OR = 0.18; 95% CI, 0.05–0.63; P = .008), and use of other drugs (OR = 0.40; 95% CI, 0.18–0.90, P = .03).. Among girls, more advanced pubertal stage was associated only with higher odds of alcohol use (OR = 1.25, 95% CI, 1.02–1.55, P = .04), and there was no significant association between age for grade, grade retention, or delayed school entry and substance use initiation. Sensitivity analyses including various combinations of these predictors yielded similar results, suggesting these findings did not result from model over-specification.
Attending more than one elementary schools (OR = 1.17 per school; 95% CI, 1.05–1.31, P = .004) and being born outside of the United States (OR = 2.83; 95% CI, 1.70–4.72, P < .001) were associated with higher odds of delayed school entry (Table 3). For race/ethnicity, being black (OR = 0.51; 95% CI, 0.34–0.80, P = .003) or Latino (OR = 0.51; 95% CI, 0.29–0.89, P = .02) was associated with lower odds of delayed school entry than being white. The odds of delayed school entry were similar for the highest-income and lowest-income families, whereas middle-income families, (ie, those earning between $25,000 and $99,000 per year) had the lowest odds of delayed school entry (OR = 0.63; 95% CI, 0.42–0.93, P = .02), compared with families earning $100,000 or more per year. Sex did not significantly predict delayed school entry.
Discussion
These findings indicate that the association between being OFG and substance use differs depending on the reason for being OFG. Delayed school entry did not confer a higher risk for substance use initiation, particularly for boys. Given that approximately 7% to 15% of US children have delayed school entry each year (3,25), these findings are reassuring for a substantial percentage of US children. However, students with delayed school entry are a heterogeneous group; further studies are needed to determine whether associations with substance use vary by reason for delayed school entry.
Pubertal stage emerged as a significant predictor of initiation of any substance use, cigarette use, and alcohol use for boys and of alcohol use initiation for girls. These results are consistent with other findings linking the timing and pace of puberty with early and midadolescent drug use (16,26) and the social and physiologic changes that accompany adolescence (27). Socially, advanced pubertal stage and early pubertal timing are associated with having more deviant peers (28). Physiologically, increased testosterone levels enhance sensation-seeking and reduce impulse control such that boys, in particular, may engage in more risky behaviors (27,29). This may explain the significant association between advanced pubertal stage and more types of substance use among boys than girls in this study. After accounting for pubertal stage, grade retention (often assumed to identify a high-risk population) was not independently associated with substance use.
These findings have important implications for substance use screening. Clinicians should consider a preadolescent’s pubertal stage, rather than merely their chronological age or school grade, when deciding at what point to initiate screening for risky health behaviors. Current recommendations to initiate substance use screening based on a child’s chronological age may result in late detection of risky behaviors for children with early or more advanced pubertal timing (3,30). For example, in this sample, 12% of fifth graders who self-identified as having a Tanner I pubertal stage reported trying at least one substance, but more than 26% of those who self-identified as having a Tanner V pubertal stage tried at least one substance. Given the importance of active screening for risky health behaviors, coupled with the negative impact of early substance use initiation, attention to pubertal stage may help clinicians avoid a missed opportunity to identify and counsel young patients at increased risk for substance use.
This study is limited by its cross-sectional design and thus did not determine whether the associations with substance use initiation vary over time or whether advanced pubertal stage causes early initiation of substance use. Although reverse causality is highly unlikely, we cannot exclude the possibility that unmeasured confounding variables are associated with both advanced pubertal stage and early initiation of substance use. Second, these findings may not generalize to a national sample, because Healthy Passages included only preadolescents from 3 large US cities and their surrounding communities. However, the cities span different regions of the country, and the sample is demographically diverse. Third, because this study focused on fifth graders, there were low rates of substance use initiation, limiting the power to detect small differences in these behaviors across groups. Thus, some of the nonsignificant findings would possibly be significant in a larger or older sample. Additionally, the drivers of substance use initiation are likely to change as students mature. However, studying substance use in this population provides insight into the age of initiation of these health behaviors and when the effects of age for grade and pubertal stage might emerge.
Despite these limitations, the findings have important implications for children’s health. Parents and education policy makers may be reassured that delayed school entry may not be a risk factor for early initiation of substance use. Additionally, the finding that pubertal stage, independent of chronological age, may be an important driver of substance use may help clinicians decide when to begin screening for risky health behaviors among preadolescent patients.
Acknowledgments
This study was supported by the Robert Wood Johnson Foundation Clinical Scholars Program and by cooperative agreements (CCU409679, CCU609653, CCU915773, U48DP000046, U48DP000057, U48DP000056, U19DP002663, U19DP002664, and U19DP002665) with the Centers for Disease Control and Prevention. The Robert Wood Johnson Foundation Clinical Scholars Program had no role in study design; collection, analysis, and interpretation of data; writing the report; or the decision to submit the report for publication. The findings and conclusions in this report are those of the authors and do not necessarily represent the official position of the Centers for Disease Control and Prevention.
Author Information
Corresponding Author: Rebecca Dudovitz, Department of Pediatrics, David Geffen School of Medicine at University of California, Los Angeles, 10833 Le Conte Ave, 12-358 CHS, MC: 175217, Los Angeles, CA 90095. Telephone: 310-794-8833. Email: rdudovitz@mednet.ucla.edu.
Author Affiliations: Paul J. Chung, University of California Los Angeles, Los Angeles, California, and RAND Corp, Santa Monica, California; Marc N. Elliott, RAND Corp, Santa Monica, California; Susan L. Davies, University of Alabama, Birmingham, Alabama; Susan Tortolero, Elizabeth Baumler, University of Texas Health Science Center, Houston, Texas; Stephen W. Banspach, Division of Adolescent and School Health, Centers for Disease Control and Prevention, Atlanta, Georgia; Mark A. Schuster, Division of General Pediatrics, Boston Children’s Hospital/Harvard Medical School, Boston, Massachusetts.
References
- Cascio E, Schanzenbach DW. First in the class? Age and the education production function. Cambridge (MA): National Bureau of Economic Research; 2007.
- Byrd RS, Weitzman M, Auinger P. Increased behavior problems associated with delayed school entry and delayed school progress. Pediatrics 1997;100(4):654–61. CrossRef PubMed
- Deming D, Dynarski S. The lengthening of childhood. Cambridge (MA): National Bureau of Economic Research; 2008.
- Verachtert P, De Fraine B, Onghena P, Ghesquière P. Season of birth and school success in the early years of primary education. Oxf Rev Educ 2010;36(3):285–306. CrossRef
- Lincove JA, Painter G. Does the age that children start kindergarten matter? Evidence of long-term educational and social outcomes. Educ Eval Policy Anal 2006;28(2):153–79. CrossRef
- Frey N. Retention, social promotion, and academic redshirting what do we know and need to know? Remedial Spec Educ 2005;26(6):332–46. CrossRef
- Byrd RS, Weitzman M, Doniger AS. Increased drug use among old-for-grade adolescents. Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med 1996;150(5):470–6. CrossRef PubMed
- Guagliardo MF, Huang Z, Hicks J, D’Angelo L. Increased drug use among old-for-grade and dropout urban adolescents. Am J Prev Med 1998;15(1):42–8. CrossRef PubMed
- Hayes DN, Hemenway D. Age-within-school-class and adolescent gun-carrying. Pediatrics 1999;103(5):e64. CrossRef PubMed
- Kuntsche E, Gmel G, Wicki M, Rehm J, Grichting E. Disentangling gender and age effects on risky single occasion drinking during adolescence. Eur J Public Health 2006;16(6):670–5. CrossRef PubMed
- Crothers LM, Schreiber JB, Schmitt AJ, Bell GR, Blasik J, Comstock LA, et al. A preliminary study of bully and victim behavior in old-for-grade students: another potential hidden cost of grade retention or delayed school entry. J Appl Sch Psychol 2010;26(4):327–38. CrossRef
- Freudenberg N, Ruglis J. Reframing school dropout as a public health issue. Prev Chronic Dis 2007;4(4):A107. PubMed
- Bassok D, Reardon SF. “Academic redshirting” in kindergarten: prevalence, patterns, and implications. Educ Eval Policy Anal 2013;35(3):283–97. CrossRef
- Schuster MA, Elliott MN, Kanouse DE, Wallander JL, Tortolero SR, Ratner JA, et al. Racial and ethnic health disparities among fifth-graders in three cities. N Engl J Med 2012;367(8):735–45. CrossRef PubMed
- Windle M, Grunbaum JA, Elliott M, Tortolero SR, Berry S, Gilliland J, et al. Healthy passages. A multilevel, multimethod longitudinal study of adolescent health. Am J Prev Med 2004;27(2):164–72. CrossRef PubMed
- Kaltiala-Heino R, Koivisto AM, Marttunen M, Fröjd S. Pubertal timing and substance use in middle adolescence: a 2-year follow-up study. J Youth Adolesc 2011;40(10):1288–1301.
- Marshall WA, Tanner JM. Variations in pattern of pubertal changes in girls. Arch Dis Child 1969;44(235):291–303. CrossRef PubMed
- Lee PA, Guo SS, Kulin HE. Age of puberty: data from the United States of America. APMIS 2001;109(2):81–8. CrossRef PubMed
- Varni JW, Seid M, Kurtin PS. PedsQL 4.0: reliability and validity of the Pediatric Quality of Life Inventory version 4.0 generic core scales in healthy and patient populations. Med Care 2001;39(8):800–12. CrossRef PubMed
- Gasper J, DeLuca S, Estacion A. Coming and going: explaining the effects of residential and school mobility on adolescent delinquency. Soc Sci Res 2010;39(3):459–76. CrossRef
- Boon HJ. School moves, coping, and achievement: models of possible interactions. J Educ Res 2011;104(1):54–70. CrossRef
- Jelleyman T, Spencer N. Residential mobility in childhood and health outcomes: a systematic review. J Epidemiol Community Health 2008;62(7):584–92. CrossRef PubMed
- Olson DH. Circumplex model of marital and family systems: assessing family functioning. Normal family processes. 2nd edition. New York (NY): Guilford Press; 1993:104–137.
- Kim S, Egerter S, Cubbin C, Takahashi ER, Braveman P. Potential implications of missing income data in population-based surveys: an example from a postpartum survey in California. Public Health Rep 2007;122(6):753–63. PubMed
- Graue ME, DiPerna J. Redshirting and early retention: who gets the “gift of time” and what are its outcomes? Am Educ Res J 2000;37(2):509–34. CrossRef
- Forbes EE, Dahl RE. Pubertal development and behavior: hormonal activation of social and motivational tendencies. Brain Cogn 2010;72(1):66–72. CrossRef PubMed
- Negriff S, Trickett PK. Peer substance use as a mediator between early pubertal timing and adolescent substance use: longitudinal associations and moderating effect of maltreatment. Drug Alcohol Depend 2012;126(1–2):95–101. CrossRef PubMed
- de Water E, Braams BR, Crone EA, Peper JS. Pubertal maturation and sex steroids are related to alcohol use in adolescents. Horm Behav 2013;63(2):392–7. CrossRef PubMed
- Hagan JF, Shaw JS, Duncan PM. Bright futures: guidelines for health supervision of infants, children, and adolescents: Elk Grove Village (IL): American Academy of Pediatrics; 2008.
Tables
Table 1. Demographic Characteristics of Old-for-Grade Fifth-Grade Participants (N = 5,147) in Healthy Passages Survey, Los Angeles, California; Birmingham, Alabama; and Houston, Texas, 2014
Characteristic | Total Sample, n = 5,129 | Grade Retainedb, n = 665 | Delayed School Entry, n = 249 | ||
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Valuea | Valuea | P Valuec | Valuea | P Valuec | |
Age, mean, y | 10.6 | 11.5 | <.001 | 11.3 | <.001 |
Male | 51.1 | 58.9 | <.001 | 55.6 | .19 |
Parental education level | |||||
Less than high school | 23.1 | 36.9 | <.001 | 25.2 | .54 |
High school | 21.4 | 29.9 | <.001 | 22.5 | .70 |
Some college | 24.8 | 19.0 | .004 | 15.6 | .003 |
College graduate | 28.9 | 11.0 | <.001 | 36.1 | .05 |
Missing data | 1.9 | 3.3 | .04 | 0.7 | .32 |
Household income | |||||
<$25,000 | 38.1 | 54.3 | <.001 | 41.0 | .46 |
$25,000–$49,000 | 24.6 | 24.3 | .83 | 16.5 | .006 |
$50,000–$99,000 | 16.6 | 8.1 | <.001 | 14.9 | .51 |
=$100,000 | 12.4 | 4.8 | <.001 | 19.0 | .005 |
Missing data | 8.6 | 8.6 | .99 | 10.1 | .33 |
Race/ethnicity | |||||
White | 22.1 | 11.6 | <.001 | 30.8 | .006 |
Black | 29.0 | 31.8 | .21 | 22.9 | .06 |
Latino | 44.5 | 54.3 | <.001 | 39.9 | .26 |
Other | 4.5 | 2.3 | <.001 | 6.5 | .10 |
Family composition | |||||
2-Parent family | 58.1 | 50.2 | <.001 | 58.0 | .98 |
Single-parent family | 37.6 | 42.6 | .01 | 38.1 | .89 |
Other | 4.3 | 7.2 | .003 | 3.9 | .77 |
No health insurance | 13.5 | 23.4 | <.001 | 18.2 | .09 |
Born outside United States | 10.1 | 14.2 | .005 | 22.4 | <.001 |
No. of elementary schools attended | 2.0 | 2.5 | <.001 | 2.3 | .001 |
Parental involvementd | 2.3 | 2.2 | <.001 | 2.3 | .91 |
School functioninge | 74.1 | 68.3 | <.001 | 72.7 | .28 |
Pubertal stagef | 2.4 | 2.7 | <.001 | 2.6 | .006 |
Substance use | |||||
Any | 17.4 | 22.1 | .005 | 15.8 | .51 |
Cigarettes | 6.6 | 10.3 | <.001 | 7.3 | .73 |
Alcohol | 5.5 | 8.3 | .01 | 5.0 | .75 |
Other drug | 9.8 | 11.8 | .11 | 8.9 | .61 |
Table 2. Association of Age for Grade With Substance Use, Fifth-Grade Participants (N = 5,147) in Healthy Passages Survey, Los Angeles, California; Birmingham, Alabama; and Houston, Texas, 2014a
Variableb | Any Substance | Cigarettes | Alcohol | Other Drug |
---|---|---|---|---|
AOR (95% CI) | AOR (95% CI) | AOR (95% CI) | AOR (95% CI) | |
Boys | ||||
Age for grade | 1.21 (0.89–1.65) | 1.38 (0.81–2.36) | 1.25 (0.81–1.93) | 0.94 (0.63–1.41) |
Grade retention | 1.03 (0.75–1.41) | 0.81 (0.47–1.38) | 1.33 (0.76–2.32) | 1.03 (0.68–1.57) |
Delayed school entry | 0.31 (0.16–0.60) | 0.46 (0.16–1.31) | 0.18 (0.05–0.63) | 0.40 (0.18–0.90) |
Pubertal stage | 1.24 (1.05–1.46) | 1.55 (1.23–1.95) | 1.32 (1.08–1.62) | 1.12 (0.92–1.37) |
Girls | ||||
Age for grade | 0.97 (0.68–1.38) | 1.27 (0.75–2.15) | 0.86 (0.54–1.37) | 0.99 (0.64–1.52) |
Grade retention | 1.01 (0.68–1.51) | 0.91 (0.50–1.68) | 1.31 (0.72–2.36) | 1.00 (0.61–1.66) |
Delayed school entry | 1.40 (0.70–2.81) | 1.54 (0.58–4.11) | 1.69 (0.63–4.54) | 1.23 (0.54–2.78) |
Pubertal stage | 1.09 (0.94–1.26) | 1.18 (0.96–1.45) | 1.25 (1.02–1.55) | 1.00 (0.82–1.23) |
Table 3. Predictors of Delayed School Entry, Fifth-Grade Participants, Healthy Passages Survey, Los Angeles, California; Birmingham, Alabama; and Houston, Texas, 2014a
Predictors | Odds Ratio (95% CI) |
---|---|
Parental involvementb | 0.90 (0.55–1.48) |
No. of elementary schools attended | 1.17 (1.05–1.31) |
Family composition | |
2-Parent household | 1 [Reference] |
Single-parent household | 1.22 (0.85–1.76) |
Other family composition | 0.96 (0.46–2.04) |
Household income | |
<$25,000 | 1 [Reference] |
$25,000–$49,999 | 0.64 (0.41–1.01) |
$50,000–$99,999 | 0.77 (0.43–1.37) |
=$100,000 | 1.14 (0.65–1.99) |
Parental level of education | |
Less than high school | 1 [Reference] |
High school | 1.17 (0.72–1.90) |
Some college | 0.63 (0.36–1.11) |
College graduate | 1.04 (0.54–2.01) |
No health insurance | 1.30 (0.84–2.01) |
Born outside United States | 2.83 (1.70–4.72) |
Male | 1.17(0.88–1.56) |
Race/ethnicity | |
White | 1 [Reference] |
Black | 0.51 (0.34–0.80) |
Latino | 0.51 (0.29–0.89) |
Other | 0.99 (0.54–1.80) |
The opinions expressed by authors contributing to this journal do not necessarily reflect the opinions of the U.S. Department of Health and Human Services, the Public Health Service, the Centers for Disease Control and Prevention, or the authors’ affiliated institutions.